Translation Watch Quarterly

نویسنده

  • Ali Darwish
چکیده

Modern languages have developed linguistic patterns that are often in consonance with the epistemic knowledge of the world. For the main part, epistemic knowledge is inferred from such linguistic patterns. In some situations however, dissonance occurs between linguistic inference and epistemic inference. When this happens, language compensates by utilizing certain linguistic patterns and rhetorical techniques to realign linguistic and epistemic realities. This paper examines aspects of translation-induced dissonance in linguistic and epistemic inference drawing on examples from news and current affairs Arabic corpus of satellite television. INTRODUCTION odern languages have developed linguistic patterns that are often in consonance with the epistemic knowledge of the world. For the main part, epistemic knowledge is inferred from such linguistic patterns and when dissonance occurs between linguistic and epistemic inferences, language compensates by utilizing certain linguistic patterns and rhetorical techniques to realign linguistic and epistemic realities. For example, the English hypothetical conditional antecedent "If I were you" is a compensatory linguistic technique to achieve concordance between linguistic and epistemic inferences when it is physically impossible for one person to be another person physically. Languages differ in their linguistic representation of epistemic knowledge and when any two languages are juxtaposed, they are bound to produce cognitive dissonance due to the disagreement that ensues between the linguistic forms within the language pair used to express the same epistemic phenomena. Left irreconciled, such infelicities are bound to change the shared experience of a speech community or surreptitiously reconstitute its social and cultural model. This paper examines aspects of translation-induced dissonance in linguistic and epistemic inference drawing on examples from news and current affairs Arabic corpus of satellite television. M TRANSLATION WATCH QUARTERLY Volume 4, Issue 1, June 2008 89 of 134 EPISTEMIC KNOWLEDGE VERSUS LINGUISTIC KNOWLEDGE Our knowledge of world’s phenomena is often expressed in linguistic-epistemic forms that express these phenomena metaphorically or define them conventionally. As our knowledge of these phenomena changes through discovery and scientific enquiry, a shift in the epistemic definitions of these phenomena occurs causing dissonance with the epistemic forms used to express them linguistically. Other epistemic forms may include pictorial and audiovisual representations of epistemic knowledge. Sherry and Trigg (1996:38) define epistemic forms as models of information. “An epistemic form is a target structure that guides the inquiry process. It shows how knowledge is organized or concepts are classified, as well as illustrating the relationships among the different facts and concepts being learned”. 1 Epistemic forms include charts, maps, process flows, that visually organize information. Figure 2— Pictorial Representation of Epistemic Reality (Picasso, Girl before a Mirror) Translation-induced Metaphoric Shift When epistemic shifts occur, the linguistic patterns in most situations do not concur with the epistemic knowledge. For example, “the sun rises” is a linguistic form that originally described a natural phenomenon as observed by people who understood it that way. While our knowledge of this phenomenon has changed— that is, we now know that the sun does not rise—the epistemic form remains in TRANSLATION WATCH QUARTERLY Volume 4, Issue 1, June 2008 90 of 134 use. To avoid this dissonance between our epistemic knowledge and linguistically represented epistemic form, the form is transferred into a metaphor that invokes a perceptual and cognitive representation consistent with the new epistemic reality, which for all intents and purposes may be in disagreement with its ontic nature. Consequently, metaphors act as re-constitutive epistemic forms that reconcile linguistic and epistemic realities. When such reconciliation occurs, metaphors become dead or dormant metaphors—they lose the idea or phenomenon they initially denoted. A good example of such metaphors is the term “heartburn”, once believed to be related to ailment of the heart, and now known to have nothing to do with the heart. Yet we continue to use the term metaphorically to refer to the burning sensation in the stomach. Heartburn (n) an uneasy burning sensation in the stomach, typically extending toward the esophagus, and sometimes associated with the eructation of an acid fluid. [American Heritage Dictionary] However, readjustment does not happen right away and a metaphoric lag persists until the cognitive epistemic schema is reset. Only then does complete reconciliation take place. This is an important aspect of metaphors because “their meanings (the ground of the metaphor) are captured by terms that are not lexically related to the lexical items in the metaphors” (Hasson and Glucksberg, 2005: 4). This property of metaphors makes them susceptible to epistemic shifts, when subjected to a translation process that focuses on the lexical items of the metaphor rather than on the terms that are related to the meaning of the metaphor. The following figure illustrates this dynamic nature of metaphors. Figure 3— Smoking Gun Metaphor: the terms that are related to the meaning of the metaphor In this connection, Searle (reported in Johnson, 1987) contends that “every literal utterance ultimately presupposes a nonrepresentational, nonpropositional, preintentional “Background” of capacities, skills, and stances in order to determine its condition of satisfaction. The meaning of any metaphor will be determined only against a preintentional background that cannot be represented propositionally” (Johnson, 1987:72). TRANSLATION WATCH QUARTERLY Volume 4, Issue 1, June 2008 91 of 134 Reinforcing this notion of metaphor, Benzon and Hays (1987) make a distinction between physiognomic and propositional representations as one between a photograph of a scene and a verbal description of the scene. They argue that the filtering or extraction process which underlies metaphor, which in the context of our discussion creates consonance between two types of epistemic representation, “is fundamentally one involving physiognomic representations. The linguistic form of the metaphor is propositional. Hence metaphor is a device for regulating the interaction of propositional and physiognomic representations, that is to say, for recognition” (59), and subsequently for creating or restoring consonance between the epistemic and linguistic realities. This semiotic status of metaphors, according to Hatim and Mason (1990:69), constitutes the crucial factor in deciding how a metaphor should be translated, since metaphoric use of language invariably conveys additional meaning. “Solutions to problems of translating metaphor should, in the first instance, be related to rhetorical function” (Hatim and Mason, 1990:233), and should seek to understand the “writer’s whole world-view” (4). This world view is actually the epistemic reality which is for the main part in congruence with the linguistic reality as expressed by the chosen epistemic form of metaphor and with the epistemic schema. Waking the Dead and Dormant Metaphors The purpose of metaphor, according to Newmark (1988) is twofold: cognitivereferential and aesthetic-pragmatic. “[i]ts referential purpose is to describe a mental process or state, a concept, a person, an object, a quality or an action more comprehensively and concisely than is possible in literal or physical language; its pragmatic purpose, which is simultaneous, is to appeal to the senses, to interest, to clarify ‘graphically’, to please, to delight, to surprise” (Newmark, 1988:104). However, beyond their textual considerations, one of the functions of metaphors, according to Kövecses (2000:17), is that they “can actually “create,” or constitute social, cultural, and psychological realities for us”. Kövecses contends that conceptual metaphors do not simply reflect cultural models; they are in fact constitutive of cultural models. It can be argued however, that metaphors do both. “For a child, for example, hearing the metaphors constructs the model; for an adult, hearing and using the metaphor reflects and constitutes the model for others”. Based on this constitutive view of metaphor, culturally bound metaphors that seek to align these realities are particularly problematic in translation. For instance, take the verb (create) in English and its metaphoric sense of “make, invent”, as in “create a peaceful environment”, “create web pages”, “create an illusion”, etc. The Arabic counterpart for the verb “create”, (���) (khalaqa), which means (1) “to create or cause to come into being from nothing”, (2) “fabricate; fake”, and (3) “to assess or evaluate” [old usage], is usually an attribute of God in sense (1). In translating English expressions comprising the word “create” into Arabic, dissonance between the epistemic knowledge and TRANSLATION WATCH QUARTERLY Volume 4, Issue 1, June 2008 92 of 134 linguistic form occurs, which is increasingly left irreconciled in daily usage of both the colloquial and standard forms, causing cognitive dissonance in alert minds when encountered, gradually changing the frequency, currency and foregrounding of the primary and secondary meanings of the verb and eventually causing metaphoric shift. Another example of this kind of dissonance is the Arabic translation of the word (astronomical) in the sense of “extremely large; exceedingly great; enormous”, as (�����) (falakiyyah) in the sense of “pertaining to astronomy”, a rendition no less moronic and retarded than the Arabic rendition of (create) which has been obstinately circulated and repeated by Arabic media producers and news editors. In this example, the concrete sense of the word (falakiyyah) is reversed, producing faulty metaphors. Faulty metaphors resulting in metaphoric shifts cause cultural and linguistic changes and mismatches of shared experience among members of the same speech community. When this happens, the likelihood of communication breakdown or misunderstanding increases.

برای دانلود متن کامل این مقاله و بیش از 32 میلیون مقاله دیگر ابتدا ثبت نام کنید

ثبت نام

اگر عضو سایت هستید لطفا وارد حساب کاربری خود شوید

منابع مشابه

An Interaction Based Composable Architecture for Building Scalable Models of Large Social, Biological, Information and Technical Systems.

High-resolution scalable models of complex socio-technical systems; i. Service-oriented architecture and delivery mechanism for facilitating the use ii. of these models by domain experts; Distributed coordinating architecture for information fusion, model execution iii. and data processing; and Scalable data management architecture and system to support model execution iv. and analytics Scalabl...

متن کامل

Spanish-Speaking Immigrants' Access to Safety Net Providers and Translation Services Across Traditional and Emerging US Destinations.

Latino immigrants have recently spread beyond traditional US enclaves to “emerging destinations.” The arrival of limited English proficiency (LEP) Spanish-speakers to these areas can challenge the health care system, as translation services may not be readily available for LEP patients. Trained auditors posed as family members of LEP patients seeking primary care in a safety net setting. We fou...

متن کامل

Cross-lingual numerical distance priming with second-language number words in native- to third-language number word translation.

An important question in research about bilingualism is whether translation from a second language (L2) to the native language (L1) is semantically mediated or whether it occurs through word associations at the lexical level. Recent research has shown that both L1 and L2 number word translation imply semantic access, suggesting strong L2 lexicosemantic mappings for number words (Duyck & Brysbae...

متن کامل

The SNARC effect in the processing of second-language number words: further evidence for strong lexico-semantic connections.

We present new evidence that word translation involves semantic mediation. It has been shown that participants react faster to small numbers with their left hand and to large numbers with their right hand. This SNARC (spatial-numerical association of response codes) effect is due to the fact that in Western cultures the semantic number line is oriented from left (small) to right (large). We obt...

متن کامل

Before babel: reflections on reading and translating freud.

The author offers some thoughts on reading and teaching Freud, on translating Freud, on translation in general, and on a possible kinship between translation and the psychoanalytic process. His reading of Freud's works, and the years he spent translating them into Hebrew and editing Hebrew editions of his writings, have made a deep and salient impression on his personal psychoanalytic palimpses...

متن کامل

ذخیره در منابع من


  با ذخیره ی این منبع در منابع من، دسترسی به آن را برای استفاده های بعدی آسان تر کنید

برای دانلود متن کامل این مقاله و بیش از 32 میلیون مقاله دیگر ابتدا ثبت نام کنید

ثبت نام

اگر عضو سایت هستید لطفا وارد حساب کاربری خود شوید

عنوان ژورنال:

دوره   شماره 

صفحات  -

تاریخ انتشار 2008